Uncovering Frankenstein’s Monster’s True
Identity
The task of defining humanity is
approachable from many different perspectives. I believe that the most
beneficial way of exploring what it means to be human is within the realm of
history, specifically referring to the Smithsonian National Museum of Natural
History’s Website, and their guidelines for humanity. Two of these guidelines,
and what I believe to be the most important ones, are that humans communicate
through language, and participate in a social life. Humans advanced
considerably over the course of history. While we were once Neanderthals almost
mindlessly wandering the Earth, we now function as a highly civilized society.
In this essay, I wish to argue that in the novel Frankenstein by Mary Shelley, Victor Frankenstein’s monster becomes
a human in the same way that our ancestors did through language and social life,
and how the convergence of those two traits further proves that he is human.
To begin, an important qualification
of humanity is the ability to express emotions through language and symbols.
Frankenstein’s monster’s progress with language is clear. When the monster
first encounters language, he describes the girl as “uttering a few sounds with
an air of melancholy” (Shelley 116). The reader can infer by the reflective
tone that although the monster cannot fully process the exchange, it intrigues
him. That curiosity urges him to listen more intently, so that he may quickly
pick up on the language. It is also evident from the above quotation that the
monster is indeed detecting emotion since he describes the sounds as “melancholy”.
His word choice here reveals that he is capable of recognizing more
sophisticated levels of emotion, compared to simply “sad” or “happy”. Also, understanding
tone of voice is important, as it accounts for a large proportion of language. If
he were not able to detect the
melancholy, then he would have absolutely no idea what feelings the girl was
expressing, as she does not actually speak.
Later, the monster attempts to learn the
language of the cottagers even more so than before since he reflects that his
“days were spent in close attention, that he might more speedily learn the
language” (Shelley 130). While dissecting the word choice, the word “close” particularly
illuminates how much the monster cares to learn the language. If the language
did not interest him, he would half-heartedly pay attention to the words, if he
even paid attention at all. Instead, he devotes full attention to the language.
This is why he is able to pick up on English so quickly, even more quickly than
a human would. Thus, this trait makes him seem borderline superhuman. Critics
might argue that this makes him more monster than man. However, a monster would
not be able to understand language at all. It is a higher-level cognitive
ability. For that reason, I think that Frankenstein’s monster is more
human-like. He also has a growing attachment to the cottagers, and wants to
further his relations with them, which leads into my next point: social
interaction.
The Smithsonian National Museum of
Natural History deems it a necessary component of humanity for the being in
question to have a social life. This is the characteristic of Frankenstein’s
monster that is the most underdeveloped. Apart from scaring off an old man just
days into his existence, the monster has not yet had any real human interaction
at this point in the novel (Shelley 114). Nevertheless, traditional humans’
social interactions over the years have also become more complex as time has gone
on, and thus the monster is moving in the right direction. He deeply engages in
observing the cottagers social interactions, such as when he sees that, “the
young stranger knelt at the old man’s feet, and would have kissed his hand, but
he raised her, and embraced her affectionately” (Shelley 128). Not only is the
monster observing the people, but he anticipates their movements, since he
guesses that the stranger almost kisses the old man’s hand. He has been
perceiving the cottagers’ interactions for so long that he learns their
individual personalities and uses that as a basis to make inferences about
their next movement. While this is very telling of his social ability, it also
says something of his tendency to do some higher-level thinking. Monsters would
only socially interact instinctively, which could put the cottagers in danger.
Frankenstein’s monster does no such thing, as he sees how the humans interact
with each other.
Additionally, the monster desires to
be accepted by the people so much that his reaction to his own reflection is
one of horror: “I had admired the perfect forms of my cottagers – their grace,
beauty, and delicate complexions: but how was I terrified when I viewed myself
in a transparent pool” (Shelley 124). Shelley uses vivid imagery here to
represent the idea that the monster considers the other humans to be superior
beings by using words with positive connotations such as grace, beauty, and
delicate. These words, while having a heavenly tone to them, also make it
apparent that the monster aspires to be like them. He has recognized that he is
different from them, and that is the first step to curing what he sees as the
problem. Additionally, the fact that he recognizes that he is different says
something about his mental capabilities. A monster normally would not care what
he looks like, but Frankenstein’s monster has higher thinking capabilities. The
monster’s terror of his appearance is rooted in his desire for peer acceptance
– a common human need. Thus, although he is not directly involved with the
other humans, he most certainly possesses the desire and emotional capability
to form relations with them, and that, after all, is the start of something
greater.
Perhaps it is more important to look
at how these two qualifications of humanity: language and social life, more
freely interact with each other to make the more complete image of a human, and
how Frankenstein’s monster is a reflection of that image. The official journal
of the American Academy of Pediatrics writes on their website that, “Children
learn language initially because they strive to connect with other persons to
share what they are feeling and thinking.” (Language Development and Emotional
Expression) The monster is very similar to a child, desperate for affection,
and possessing a strong need for social interaction. The monster finds that the
cottagers, “possessed a method of communicating their experience and feeling to
one another by articulate sounds”, and he “ardently desired to become
acquainted” with this “godlike science” (Shelley 121 - 122). Language is a tool
for the monster, a means of gratification for his social need. What the monster
and child have in common is that they are both just starting out in life. They
learn to utilize language so that they can interact with each other on a deeper
level. The monster especially considers language important, because he uses the
word “godlike”. Comparing language to a god sets it on a level that is far
above anything else. He clings to it because it is the solution to his
loneliness. The monster also “ardently” wants to learn the language, because he
recognizes that language is an important bridge between himself and the
cottagers. All of these examples converge together to form the theme that
language is the single most important tool in social interaction. Without it,
humans would have to resort to using only body language and facial expressions
for communication. If that were the case, then humans would not be much more of
an intelligible species than monsters. However, humans can utilize the power of
speech, and so they are able to make connections with people that would be
utterly unimaginable to the monster mind.
Emotion is another strong link
between language and social interaction. Emotions help humans to better
understand each other, as well as remember what has transpired in a social
gathering. Thus, the connections are more legitimate with a solid foundation of
memories. Again, the Official Journal of the American Pediatrics writes that “word
learning is intimately connected to a child's emotional life, because infants
learn language to talk about and thereby to share those things that they are
thinking and feeling: the persons, objects, and events that make up the goals
and situations in everyday events that are the causes and circumstances of
emotion” (Language Development and Emotional Expression). Frankenstein’s
monster displays this emotional capability while watching the cottagers
interactions, “The gentle manners and beauty of the cottagers greatly endeared
them to me: when they were unhappy, I felt depressed; when they rejoiced, I
sympathised in their joys” (Shelley 122). Before the monster can learn
language, although at this point he is already trying, he still finds an
emotional connection with the cottagers through his observation of them. Since he feels their depression and their joy,
according to the Official Journal of the American Pediatrics, he should be able
to process language easier. By the word “sympathised” it is evident that the
monster goes beyond merely feeling joy by himself, but is able to feel joy with
the cottagers, and thus forms a connection with them, even though they still
are not aware of his existence. Also, since the monster lists several feelings,
it shows that he has the capacity to feel a diverse range of emotions, making
him become more human.
Conclusively, Frankenstein’s monster
should be considered human, or very near human, based on the fact that he
understands language, and has the desire to have social interactions with
people. He is also well on his way to accomplishing the latter. Although he may
be considered human from a historical standpoint, his appearance no doubt
affects others abilities to see him as human, which consequentially creates
problems for the monster. The reader can sympathize with the monster, as he applies
emotions into almost everything he does, just like his fellow human
counterparts.
Works Cited
"Human
Characteristics: What Does It Mean to Be Human." Human Evolution by The
Smithsonian Institution's Human Origins
Program. Smithsonian
National Museum of Natural History, n.d. Web. 14 Jan. 2014.
"Language
Development and Emotional Expression." Language Development and
Emotional Expression. American Academy of Pediatrics, 1998.
Web. 13 Feb. 2014.
Shelley,
Mary Wollstonecraft, and Lynd Ward. Frankenstein: The Lynd Ward Illustrated
Edition. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications, 2009. Print.
1 comment:
Your introduction is basically good and clear, although the line about Neanderthals is silly (because it's so wrong). I particularly like your focus on the *convergence* of language & social life, which I think has a lot of potential to lead to an effective essay.
I think your discussion of whether or not his ability with language is superhuman is a little fuzzy, and could have been developed. "He also has a growing attachment to the cottagers, and wants to further his relations with them, which leads into my next point: social interaction." I think you're basically arguing that because he desires language in a human way, in a human context, that makes him human, regardless of how quickly he learns. I can be on board with that - it just seems a little underdeveloped to me.
I wish that your close attention to the details of how the Monster listens to and watches the DeLaceys would have carried over into a close analysis of exactly how he *does* interact with the old man. His attempt to seek protection from the old man, in particular, would do a lot for your argument.
You have a long, thoughtful and well-conceived paragraph on how we see the monster wants language in order to achieve social interaction: "All of these examples converge together to form the theme that language is the single most important tool in social interaction." This is all good material, but I do have questions/problems. First, can we easily call him human if none of the social interaction actually happens? What happens, in other words, when the humanity of a child or monster is *denied* by those he/she is interacting with? Second, your research is effective, but it raises something interesting. What does it mean that Shelley can be understood so readily through *contemporary* theories of childhood and language development? Third, what can you use here to apply to Victor's own interaction with the monster, e.g., on top of the mountain?
Those questions, in fact, summarize my response to the ending of your essay. The essay doesn't end badly, by any means, but it's also really incomplete - by narrowly focusing on the monster's interactions with the Delaceys, you constrict yourself when you should be expanding from what we might call the monster's childhood into his adulthood. I'm not saying that there's anything wrong about your approach - it's just that even if you're rooting your argument in his Delacey period, you can't just ignore his interactions with Victor and Walton. Failing to do so makes the ending of the essay seem a little repetitive, even stuck, when the analysis could be moving forward.
Your research could have been expanded, too. You effectively use what you have, but a more academic source about language development (or Frankenstein, or both) would have been good.
Post a Comment